来自用户 muton 的文献。
当前共找到 29 篇文献分享,本页显示第 1 - 20 篇。
1.
muton
(2024-11-30 21:53):
#paper An abstract relational map emerges in the human medial prefrontal cortex with consolidation. doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.10.11.617652 我们在面对一个问题时,如果能够识别问题背后的结构,那么对于解决这个问题,以及面对类似问题时候的泛化是及其有帮助的。最近的理论研究强调了与感官细节完全分离的显式结构表征对于泛化的有用性。啮齿动物的研究表明,这种结构的抽象是逐渐发生的,随着时间的推移在大脑皮层中进行。然而,在人类中这种显式关系表征的直接证据很少,它与巩固机制的关系尚未研究清楚。因此,作者使用一种图形学习的范式在人类内侧前额叶皮质中找到了这样一个关系图。重要的是,这种表征在学习后早期并没有表现出来,但在几天后却出现了。说明这种抽象的关系结构图是逐渐形成并抽象化的,并且最终存储在内侧前额叶。这一结果为理解人类强大的推理能力提供了新的视角。
bioRxiv,
2024-10-11.
DOI: 10.1101/2024.10.11.617652
Abstract:
AbstractUnderstanding the structure of a problem, such as the relationships between stimuli, supports fast learning and flexible reasoning. Recent theoretical suggestions have highlighted the usefulness of explicit structural representations that …
>>>
AbstractUnderstanding the structure of a problem, such as the relationships between stimuli, supports fast learning and flexible reasoning. Recent theoretical suggestions have highlighted the usefulness of explicit structural representations that are fully divorced from sensory details for generalisation. Rodent work has suggested that abstraction of structure occurs gradually, over time, in cortex. However, direct evidence of such explicit relational representations in humans is scarce, and its relationship to consolidation mechanisms is underexplored. Here, we use a graph-learning paradigm to find such a relational map in the human medial prefrontal cortex. Importantly, this representation was absent early after learning but emerged on the time scale of days. These results shed new light on neural representations underlying the remarkable human ability to draw accurate inferences from little data.
<<<
翻译
2.
muton
(2024-10-31 23:48):
#paper:doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.08.29.505742 Parallel cognitive maps for short-term statistical and long-term semantic relationships in the hippocampal formation 海马-内嗅皮层不仅加工空间信息,同时也加工其它类型的信息,如关系信息(社交信息)等。但是海马到底是把刺激的不同维度整合到一个联合地图中还是每个信息纬度都是一个平行地图?作者重新分析了之前Garvert等的核磁数据,实验任务可以构建出一个包含语义信息和统计规律的地图,作者通过计算模型,mds等方法计算证明了海马中是形成了多个地图的,并不是将多个结构整合到一个地图中。
bioRxiv,
2022-8-29.
DOI: 10.1101/2022.08.29.505742
Abstract:
AbstractThe hippocampal-entorhinal system uses cognitive maps to represent spatial knowledge and other types of relational information, such as the transition probabilities between objects. However, objects can often be characterized in …
>>>
AbstractThe hippocampal-entorhinal system uses cognitive maps to represent spatial knowledge and other types of relational information, such as the transition probabilities between objects. However, objects can often be characterized in terms of different types of relations simultaneously, e.g. semantic similarities learned over the course of a lifetime as well as transitions experienced over a brief timeframe in an experimental setting. Here we ask how the hippocampal formation handles the embedding of stimuli in multiple relational structures that differ vastly in terms of their mode and timescale of acquisition: Does it integrate the different stimulus dimensions into one conjunctive map, or is each dimension represented in a parallel map? To this end, we reanalyzed functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from Garvert et al. (2017) that had previously revealed an entorhinal map which coded for newly learnt statistical regularities. We used a triplet odd-one-out task to construct a semantic distance matrix for presented items and applied fMRI adaptation analysis to show that the degree of similarity of representations in bilateral hippocampus decreases as a function of semantic distance between presented objects. Importantly, while both maps localize to the hippocampal formation, this semantic map is anatomically distinct from the originally described entorhinal map. This finding supports the idea that the hippocampal-entorhinal system forms parallel cognitive maps reflecting the embedding of objects in diverse relational structures.
<<<
翻译
3.
muton
(2024-09-30 23:40):
#paper https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-07973-1 Human hippocampal and entorhinal neurons encode the temporal structure of experience.
从经验中提取出深层的时序结构是学习和记忆的核心,它使我们能够预测接下来可能发生的事情。作者记录了颅内电极个体的单个神经元活动,并发现人类海马和齿状回神经元会逐渐调整其活动,用以编码复杂图像呈现序列的时序结构。这种表征会迅速形成,无需向被试提供具体指令,并且在规则不再存在的情况下仍然存在。此外,从海马-齿状回神经元群体活动的结构与定义序列的结构图非常相似,同时,也反映了即将到来的刺激的概率。最后,学习序列图与个体神经元自发地、压缩时间地重放与其先前经历的图轨迹相对应的活动有关。这些发现表明,海马体和齿状回的神经元整合“什么”和“何时”的信息,以提取人类经历的持久且可预测的时间结构表征。
Nature,
2024-9-25.
DOI: 10.1038/s41586-024-07973-1
Abstract:
AbstractExtracting the underlying temporal structure of experience is a fundamental aspect of learning and memory that allows us to predict what is likely to happen next. Current knowledge about the …
>>>
AbstractExtracting the underlying temporal structure of experience is a fundamental aspect of learning and memory that allows us to predict what is likely to happen next. Current knowledge about the neural underpinnings of this cognitive process in humans stems from functional neuroimaging research1–5. As these methods lack direct access to the neuronal level, it remains unknown how this process is computed by neurons in the human brain. Here we record from single neurons in individuals who have been implanted with intracranial electrodes for clinical reasons, and show that human hippocampal and entorhinal neurons gradually modify their activity to encode the temporal structure of a complex image presentation sequence. This representation was formed rapidly, without providing specific instructions to the participants, and persisted when the prescribed experience was no longer present. Furthermore, the structure recovered from the population activity of hippocampal–entorhinal neurons closely resembled the structural graph defining the sequence, but at the same time, also reflected the probability of upcoming stimuli. Finally, learning of the sequence graph was related to spontaneous, time-compressed replay of individual neurons’ activity corresponding to previously experienced graph trajectories. These findings demonstrate that neurons in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex integrate the ‘what’ and ‘when’ information to extract durable and predictive representations of the temporal structure of human experience.
<<<
翻译
4.
muton
(2024-08-31 23:32):
#paper Huang, Q., Xiao, Z., Yu, Q. et al. Replay-triggered brain-wide activation in humans. Nat Commun 15, 7185 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-51582-5 将零散的经历整合成一个连贯的故事可以塑造认知地图,为我们的经历提供有组织的认知表征。在这个过程中,过去的记忆会被激活并以顺序播放,促进海马体与皮质之间的对话。然而,记忆序列激活(或重播)同时在整个大脑范围内发生的协同作用仍尚未得到充分研究。在这项研究中,作者利用EEG-fMRI同时记录技术捕捉记忆重播的时空动态。我们发现,在心理模拟过程中,过去的记忆会以快速序列的形式通过EEG检测到。这些短暂的重播事件与海马体和前额叶皮质的增强fMRI活动有关。重播发生的频率会增强海马体与默认模式网络(一组对代表认知地图的关键脑区)之间的功能连接。另一方面,当受试者在学习后休息时,与任务相关的记忆重播强度高于学习前的休息时,并与海马体激活的增强和海马体与齿状回的连接增强有关。我们的研究结果共同表明,短暂的记忆重现与连续的重放过程与大脑广泛的区域性活动有关。
Nature communications,
2024-Aug-21.
DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-51582-5
PMID: 39169063
PMCID:PMC11339350
Abstract:
The consolidation of discrete experiences into a coherent narrative shapes the cognitive map, providing structured mental representations of our experiences. In this process, past memories are reactivated and replayed in …
>>>
The consolidation of discrete experiences into a coherent narrative shapes the cognitive map, providing structured mental representations of our experiences. In this process, past memories are reactivated and replayed in sequence, fostering hippocampal-cortical dialogue. However, brain-wide engagement coinciding with sequential reactivation (or replay) of memories remains largely unexplored. In this study, employing simultaneous EEG-fMRI, we capture both the spatial and temporal dynamics of memory replay. We find that during mental simulation, past memories are replayed in fast sequences as detected via EEG. These transient replay events are associated with heightened fMRI activity in the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Replay occurrence strengthens functional connectivity between the hippocampus and the default mode network, a set of brain regions key to representing the cognitive map. On the other hand, when subjects are at rest following learning, memory reactivation of task-related items is stronger than that of pre-learning rest, and is also associated with heightened hippocampal activation and augmented hippocampal connectivity to the entorhinal cortex. Together, our findings highlight a distributed, brain-wide engagement associated with transient memory reactivation and its sequential replay.
<<<
翻译
5.
muton
(2024-07-31 18:12):
#paper:Hu, H., Li, A., Zhang, L., Liu, C., Shi, L., Peng, X., ... & Xue, G. (2024). Goal-directed attention transforms both working and long-term memory representations in the human parietal cortex. PLoS biology, 22(7), e3002721. 日常生活中我们会加工诸多信息,并存储到长时记忆,但当多个信息同时存在时,选择性注意是如何处理相互竞争的信息,减少干扰信息的影响呢。现有的工作记忆研究发现,注意可以通过灵活的编码和映射机制来保护目标表征。然而,在更复杂的图片刺激以及更长时间尺度下,这些机制是否同样有助于目标表征的记忆编码和长时提取尚未清楚。这项研究结合选择性注意任务和一天后的再认任务,揭示了选择性注意影响工作记忆以及长时记忆的神经机制。首先,研究采用分类器解码的分析方法,发现知觉注意和内省注意两种自上而下的注意在有效线索出现后均能增强目标表征和抑制干扰表征,且知觉注意的调节作用更强。此外,与视觉皮层相比,顶叶对干扰项的抑制作用更强,在知觉注意条件下干扰项的解码概率甚至和未出现的图片种类的解码概率无显著差异,说明顶叶更能抵抗干扰,更受自上而下的注意调节影响。并且虽然顶叶和视觉皮层都表征了目标信息,但在有干扰图片的知觉注意条件下,目标信息更多表征在背外侧顶叶,而在没有干扰物的基线条件下更多表征在视觉皮层。这表明在面对干扰时,大脑中维持目标表征的区域发生了部分的转移。研究还发现,在再认阶段目标表征和干扰表征的相似性越低,被试的总体记忆成绩越好。这些结果表示人类在进行长时记忆加工过程中受注意调控的影响,也证明了情景记忆的动态性。
IF:7.800Q1
PLoS biology,
2024-Jul.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002721
PMID: 39008524
PMCID:PMC11271952
Abstract:
The abundance of distractors in the world poses a major challenge to our brain's limited processing capacity, but little is known about how selective attention modulates stimulus representations in the …
>>>
The abundance of distractors in the world poses a major challenge to our brain's limited processing capacity, but little is known about how selective attention modulates stimulus representations in the brain to reduce interference and support durable target memory. Here, we collected functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data in a selective attention task in which target and distractor pictures of different visual categories were simultaneously presented. Participants were asked to selectively process the target according to the effective cue, either before the encoding period (i.e., perceptual attention) or the maintenance period (i.e., reflective attention). On the next day, participants were asked to perform a memory recognition task in the scanner in which the targets, distractors, and novel items were presented in a pseudorandom order. Behavioral results showed that perceptual attention was better at enhancing target memory and reducing distractor memory than reflective attention, although the overall memory capacity (memory for both target and distractor) was comparable. Using multiple-voxel pattern analysis of the neural data, we found more robust target representation and weaker distractor representation in working memory for perceptual attention than for reflective attention. Interestingly, perceptual attention partially shifted the regions involved in maintaining the target representation from the visual cortex to the parietal cortex. Furthermore, the targets and distractors simultaneously presented in the perceptual attention condition showed reduced pattern similarity in the parietal cortex during retrieval compared to items not presented together. This neural pattern repulsion positively correlated with individuals' recognition of both targets and distractors. These results emphasize the critical role of selective attention in transforming memory representations to reduce interference and improve long-term memory performance.
<<<
翻译
6.
muton
(2024-06-30 23:00):
#paper Alagapan, S., Choi, K. S., Heisig, S., Riva-Posse, P., Crowell, A., Tiruvadi, V., Obatusin, M., Veerakumar, A., Waters, A. C., Gross, R. E., Quinn, S., Denison, L., O'Shaughnessy, M., Connor, M., Canal, G., Cha, J., Hershenberg, R., Nauvel, T., Isbaine, F., Afzal, M. F., … Rozell, C. J. (2023). Cingulate dynamics track depression recovery with deep brain stimulation. Nature, 622(7981), 130–138. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06541-3 扣带下扣带回(SCC)的脑深部刺激(DBS)可以为难治性抑郁症(TRD)提供长期的症状缓解,但是不一定能够实现稳定的康复,但是作者使用了一种新的设备叫做SCC DBS,记录了抑郁症患者24周的治疗成果,结果发现90%的患者表现出了稳定的临床响应,70%的患者症状达到缓解。用其中6个人的局部场电位结合AI可以达到识别SCC局部场电位变化进而预测病人当下的临床状态。总体来说,作者使用一种新的电刺激技术和手段大幅成功治愈了抑郁症患者,且发现了抑郁状态的神经指标。
Abstract:
Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subcallosal cingulate (SCC) can provide long-term symptom relief for treatment-resistant depression (TRD). However, achieving stable recovery is unpredictable, typically requiring trial-and-error stimulation adjustments due …
>>>
Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subcallosal cingulate (SCC) can provide long-term symptom relief for treatment-resistant depression (TRD). However, achieving stable recovery is unpredictable, typically requiring trial-and-error stimulation adjustments due to individual recovery trajectories and subjective symptom reporting. We currently lack objective brain-based biomarkers to guide clinical decisions by distinguishing natural transient mood fluctuations from situations requiring intervention. To address this gap, we used a new device enabling electrophysiology recording to deliver SCC DBS to ten TRD participants (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT01984710). At the study endpoint of 24 weeks, 90% of participants demonstrated robust clinical response, and 70% achieved remission. Using SCC local field potentials available from six participants, we deployed an explainable artificial intelligence approach to identify SCC local field potential changes indicating the patient's current clinical state. This biomarker is distinct from transient stimulation effects, sensitive to therapeutic adjustments and accurate at capturing individual recovery states. Variable recovery trajectories are predicted by the degree of preoperative damage to the structural integrity and functional connectivity within the targeted white matter treatment network, and are matched by objective facial expression changes detected using data-driven video analysis. Our results demonstrate the utility of objective biomarkers in the management of personalized SCC DBS and provide new insight into the relationship between multifaceted (functional, anatomical and behavioural) features of TRD pathology, motivating further research into causes of variability in depression treatment.
<<<
翻译
7.
muton
(2024-05-31 23:28):
#paper DOI: 10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7 Similar neural responses predict friendship 一个很有意思的研究,当我们处在群体当中时,我们有很明显的同质性趋向,另外个体也更倾向于和与自己更相似的人交朋友。那么朋友之间的相似性是否反映了我们在感知、解释和应对世界方面的更深层次的相似性?作者为了测试社交网络相似性与心理反应相似性的关系,使用功能性磁共振成像来扫描受试者在自由观看自然主义电影时的大脑。结果发现,朋友之间观看视听电影时的神经反应非常相似,并且在现实世界的社交网络中,这种相似性随着距离的增加而降低。这表明你和你的朋友越像你们在面对同一个事物的神经相似性可能也越高。
Abstract:
Human social networks are overwhelmingly homophilous: individuals tend to befriend others who are similar to them in terms of a range of physical attributes (e.g., age, gender). Do similarities among …
>>>
Human social networks are overwhelmingly homophilous: individuals tend to befriend others who are similar to them in terms of a range of physical attributes (e.g., age, gender). Do similarities among friends reflect deeper similarities in how we perceive, interpret, and respond to the world? To test whether friendship, and more generally, social network proximity, is associated with increased similarity of real-time mental responding, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to scan subjects' brains during free viewing of naturalistic movies. Here we show evidence for neural homophily: neural responses when viewing audiovisual movies are exceptionally similar among friends, and that similarity decreases with increasing distance in a real-world social network. These results suggest that we are exceptionally similar to our friends in how we perceive and respond to the world around us, which has implications for interpersonal influence and attraction.
<<<
翻译
8.
muton
(2024-04-30 22:07):
#paper doi: 10.1126/science.adk8261. Epub 2024 Mar 28. Selection of experience for memory by hippocampal sharp wave ripples 记忆对人类来说是非常重要的,但是并不是所有经历过的事件都能够被记住,那么大脑是如何筛选出值得记住的瞬间的呢? 纽约大学的György Buzsáki教授发现一种神经振荡的信号叫做SWR,也就是尖波涟漪波,他们认为清醒时期尖波涟漪波的发放会帮助记忆“打标”,帮助我们从事件中选择出有用的部分,并且在本文中的设想是清醒时起有尖波涟漪波发放附近的事件在睡眠时期会同样发放SWR并以replay的形式进行记忆巩固,帮助增强了记忆。本文运用了序列非负矩阵分解(seqNMF) 与统一流形的估计投影技术(UMAP)帮助解码了小鼠在迷宫路径探索下的行为和时间信息,使这一假设得到了数据的支持。
Abstract:
Experiences need to be tagged during learning for further consolidation. However, neurophysiological mechanisms that select experiences for lasting memory are not known. By combining large-scale neural recordings in mice with …
>>>
Experiences need to be tagged during learning for further consolidation. However, neurophysiological mechanisms that select experiences for lasting memory are not known. By combining large-scale neural recordings in mice with dimensionality reduction techniques, we observed that successive maze traversals were tracked by continuously drifting populations of neurons, providing neuronal signatures of both places visited and events encountered. When the brain state changed during reward consumption, sharp wave ripples (SPW-Rs) occurred on some trials, and their specific spike content decoded the trial blocks that surrounded them. During postexperience sleep, SPW-Rs continued to replay those trial blocks that were reactivated most frequently during waking SPW-Rs. Replay content of awake SPW-Rs may thus provide a neurophysiological tagging mechanism to select aspects of experience that are preserved and consolidated for future use.
<<<
翻译
9.
muton
(2024-03-31 23:40):
#paper doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-024-00930-y 非常有意思的发现👍 有点像锻炼肌肉,首先撕裂肌肉细胞,然后修复。大脑要形成强的突触联系,也需要先破坏DNA,然后修复。
Abstract:
No abstract available.
10.
muton
(2024-02-28 23:02):
#paper The Conceptual Structure of Human Relationships Across Modern and Historical Cultures preprint 人类社会复杂性的特征就是关系的复杂性,我们会和家庭、学校、工作、社区甚至社交网络的各类人群建立不同的关系。但我们应该如何理解如此复杂的人际关系系统?通过使用自然语言处理(NLP)、在线调查、实验室认知任务和计算建模,对世界各地的各种现代文化进行研究。(n = 20425)和跨越3,000年历史的古代文化,作者发现了关系概念的普遍表征空间,由五个主要维度组成(正式、主动、效价、交换、平等)和三个核心范畴(敌对、公共和私人关系)。这一工作推进了对人类社会性的理解。并且通过比较不同国家文化差异,作者发现中美存在巨大文化距离,在理解人际关系中的亲密程度时,美国人似乎更关注物理距离,而中国人更关注心理距离。
PsyArXiv Preprints,
2023.
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/ut6qp
Abstract:
A defining characteristic of social complexity in Homo sapiens is the diversity of our relationships. We build various types of connections with people in families, schools, workplaces, neighborhoods, and online …
>>>
A defining characteristic of social complexity in Homo sapiens is the diversity of our relationships. We build various types of connections with people in families, schools, workplaces, neighborhoods, and online communities. How do we make sense of such complex systems of human relationships? By using natural language processing, online surveys, laboratory cognitive tasks, and computational modelling on diverse modern cultures across the world (n = 20,425) and ancient cultures across 3,000 years of history, we discovered a universal representational space of relationship concepts, comprised of five principal dimensions (formality, activeness, valence, exchange, equality) and three core categories (hostile, public and private relationships). Our work reveals the fundamental cognitive constructs and cultural principles of relationship knowledge and advances our understanding of human sociality.
<<<
翻译
11.
muton
(2024-01-31 23:04):
# paper:DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abj4383 Emerged human-like facial expression representation in a deep convolutional neural network 最近的研究发现,经过训练以识别面部身份的深度卷积神经网络(DCNN)自发地学习了支持面部表情识别的特征,反之亦然。作者通过比较pretrain的VGG-Face,untrained VGG-Face以及VGG 16三个模型发现,只有pretrain的VGG-Face最后一层的1.25%的units表现出了和人类类似的面部表情识别以及表情混淆的特征。这些研究结果揭示了特定单元的面孔识别视觉经验对面孔表情知觉发展的必要性。
Abstract:
Recent studies found that the deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) trained to recognize facial identities spontaneously learned features that support facial expression recognition, and vice versa. Here, we showed that …
>>>
Recent studies found that the deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) trained to recognize facial identities spontaneously learned features that support facial expression recognition, and vice versa. Here, we showed that the self-emerged expression-selective units in a VGG-Face trained for facial identification were tuned to distinct basic expressions and, importantly, exhibited hallmarks of human expression recognition (i.e., facial expression confusion and categorical perception). We then investigated whether the emergence of expression-selective units is attributed to either face-specific experience or domain-general processing by conducting the same analysis on a VGG-16 trained for object classification and an untrained VGG-Face without any visual experience, both having the identical architecture with the pretrained VGG-Face. Although similar expression-selective units were found in both DCNNs, they did not exhibit reliable human-like characteristics of facial expression perception. Together, these findings revealed the necessity of domain-specific visual experience of face identity for the development of facial expression perception, highlighting the contribution of nurture to form human-like facial expression perception.
<<<
翻译
12.
muton
(2023-12-31 13:31):
#paper Theta mediated dynamics ofhuman hippocampal-neocortical learning systems in memory formation and retrieval https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-44011-6 本研究探讨了人类海马-皮层学习系统在记忆编码和提取过程中的θ节律动力学。研究结果表明,θ节律在记忆形成和检索过程中起着重要的调节作用,并且海马和新皮质之间存在信息传递的动态变化。这些发现对于理解记忆的神经机制和相关疾病具有重要意义。具体而言,文章关心三个研究问题:1)在记忆编码和检索过程中,海马和新皮质之间的信息传递方向是如何的?2)在不同条件下,海马和新皮质之间的信息传递是否存在差异?3)Theta频率范围内的神经振荡是否在海马-新皮质学习系统中发挥重要作用?作者记录了8个颅内病人数据,通过模式分离任务(让被试判断old,lure,new)中的电生理信号来揭示4-5hz的θ节律分别在编码和提取过程的作用,以及这一节律如何贡献于海马和皮层的信息交互。结果发现,4-5hz的θ节律在模式完成和模式分离中表现不同,海马和新皮质无论在模式完成还是模式分离还是编码提取阶段都表现出双向信息交流。但这几种条件下存在一定的偏向性,如模式分离(即能够区分相似的项目)4 - 5hz介导了新皮层→海马方向偏差,而如果4-5hz可以在编码阶段介导海马→新皮层方向偏差,那么被试在提取阶段可以更好的识别学过的项目。总体来说海马和新皮层的交互在记忆编码提取以及模式分离和模式整合等不同阶段和条件下展现出了非常动态性的过程,4-5hz的θ震荡在其中起到一定的作用。
Abstract:
Episodic memory arises as a function of dynamic interactions between the hippocampus and the neocortex, yet the mechanisms have remained elusive. Here, using human intracranial recordings during a mnemonic discrimination …
>>>
Episodic memory arises as a function of dynamic interactions between the hippocampus and the neocortex, yet the mechanisms have remained elusive. Here, using human intracranial recordings during a mnemonic discrimination task, we report that 4-5 Hz (theta) power is differentially recruited during discrimination vs. overgeneralization, and its phase supports hippocampal-neocortical when memories are being formed and correctly retrieved. Interactions were largely bidirectional, with small but significant net directional biases; a hippocampus-to-neocortex bias during acquisition of new information that was subsequently correctly discriminated, and a neocortex-to-hippocampus bias during accurate discrimination of new stimuli from similar previously learned stimuli. The 4-5 Hz rhythm may facilitate the initial stages of information acquisition by neocortex during learning and the recall of stored information from cortex during retrieval. Future work should further probe these dynamics across different types of tasks and stimuli and computational models may need to be expanded accordingly to accommodate these findings.
<<<
翻译
13.
muton
(2023-11-30 23:05):
#paper: Schonhaut, D. R., Aghajan, Z. M., Kahana, M. J., & Fried, I. (2023). A neural code for time and space in the human brain. Cell Reports, 42(11). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2023.113238 时间和空间对人类的记忆来说是非常重要的两个维度,不仅可以帮助构建我们的经验,也可以帮助我们预测未来。以往有部分研究发现了时间和空间的神经关联,比如时间细胞和位置细胞的发现等。但是这两个维度是如何在个体记忆中被整合的仍未可知。作者基于这一问题记录了在定时性空间导航虚拟游戏中10个病人的单细胞放电情况,任务分为延迟等待阶段,寻金时间(编码阶段),下一个延迟等待阶段和挖金阶段(提取阶段)。结果发现,内侧颞叶和前额叶皮层神经元在无任务延迟期间编码时间信息,在空间探索过程中,时间和位置是独立表征的,时间细胞会在相似的事件下重新编码(remap)但是位置细胞仍会以稳定的模式放电,群体神经活动代表序列中多个事件的时间信息。本文的亮点在于首次在人类中同时研究了时间和空间信息在记忆形成中的放电特征。
Abstract:
Time and space are primary dimensions of human experience. Separate lines of investigation have identified neural correlates of time and space, yet little is known about how these representations converge …
>>>
Time and space are primary dimensions of human experience. Separate lines of investigation have identified neural correlates of time and space, yet little is known about how these representations converge during self-guided experience. Here, 10 subjects with intracranially implanted microelectrodes play a timed, virtual navigation game featuring object search and retrieval tasks separated by fixed delays. Time cells and place cells activate in parallel during timed navigation intervals, whereas a separate time cell sequence spans inter-task delays. The prevalence, firing rates, and behavioral coding strengths of time cells and place cells are indistinguishable-yet time cells selectively remap between search and retrieval tasks, while place cell responses remain stable. Thus, the brain can represent time and space as overlapping but dissociable dimensions. Time cells and place cells may constitute a biological basis for the cognitive map of spatiotemporal context onto which memories are written.
<<<
翻译
14.
muton
(2023-10-31 22:43):
#paper: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2304085120 Modeling naturalistic face processing in humans with deep convolutional neural networks 大脑在加工信息的过程中,都是一个动态变化的过程,以往使用深度卷积神经网络可以模拟出大脑在记忆过程中的动态变化过程,但是对人脸材料而言,都是基于静态的材料进行解码,少有研究使用深度卷积神经网络的方法来解释大脑加工人脸的过程。由于人脸对于人类而言是具有特殊属性的一类材料,并且具有多维属性,如性别,表情,年龄等等,使用此方法解码是非常必要的。本文中作者使用700多个陌生面孔的自然刺激,每个视频长达4s,结合行为以及FMRI成像数据,对人脸加工过程进行了解码,结果发现,深度卷积神经网络模型在中间层/阶段可以很好的解码出分类情况,此阶段与行为结果也一致(行为结果更多体现出了分类信息),但是在全连接层可能更多体现了其他认知过程,如个性化信息等,神经信号的结果包含了更多动态和其他信息加工的信息。因此此模型可以很好的预测面部分类情况,但是对于随后的认知情况和动态变化情况并不能有一个很好的分类效果,因此,解码人脸动态加工过程的深度卷积神经网络模型仍有待进一步发展。
IF:9.400Q1
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
2023-10-24.
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2304085120
PMID: 37847731
Abstract:
Deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) trained for face identification can rival and even exceed human-level performance. The ways in which the internal face representations in DCNNs relate to human cognitive …
>>>
Deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) trained for face identification can rival and even exceed human-level performance. The ways in which the internal face representations in DCNNs relate to human cognitive representations and brain activity are not well understood. Nearly all previous studies focused on static face image processing with rapid display times and ignored the processing of naturalistic, dynamic information. To address this gap, we developed the largest naturalistic dynamic face stimulus set in human neuroimaging research (700+ naturalistic video clips of unfamiliar faces). We used this naturalistic dataset to compare representational geometries estimated from DCNNs, behavioral responses, and brain responses. We found that DCNN representational geometries were consistent across architectures, cognitive representational geometries were consistent across raters in a behavioral arrangement task, and neural representational geometries in face areas were consistent across brains. Representational geometries in late, fully connected DCNN layers, which are optimized for individuation, were much more weakly correlated with cognitive and neural geometries than were geometries in late-intermediate layers. The late-intermediate face-DCNN layers successfully matched cognitive representational geometries, as measured with a behavioral arrangement task that primarily reflected categorical attributes, and correlated with neural representational geometries in known face-selective topographies. Our study suggests that current DCNNs successfully capture neural cognitive processes for categorical attributes of faces but less accurately capture individuation and dynamic features.
<<<
翻译
15.
muton
(2023-09-30 22:08):
#paper Geissmann, L., Coynel, D., Papassotiropoulos, A., & de Quervain, D. J. (2023). Neurofunctional underpinnings of individual differences in visual episodic memory performance. Nature Communications, 14(1), 5694. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-41380-w 情景记忆在个体之间存在着很大的差异。以往核磁研究的证据证明了在群体水平上可以成功编码记忆的脑区,但是在解释个体差异方面的脑区和机制还并未阐明。作者利用大数据集,分析了1498名被试在图片编码任务中的核磁结果,他们发现海马体、眶额皮层和后扣带皮层反应性的个体差异解释了情景记忆表现的个体差异。而枕叶外侧皮层等,与成功的记忆编码有关,但与个体的记忆差异无关。这个研究很有趣,因为一直以来关于记忆的研究都集中于怎么记住,很少关注个体差异,对于个体差异的关注对于理解个体记性好坏具有一定的启示作用。
Abstract:
Episodic memory, the ability to consciously recollect information and its context, varies substantially among individuals. While prior fMRI studies have identified certain brain regions linked to successful memory encoding at …
>>>
Episodic memory, the ability to consciously recollect information and its context, varies substantially among individuals. While prior fMRI studies have identified certain brain regions linked to successful memory encoding at a group level, their role in explaining individual memory differences remains largely unexplored. Here, we analyze fMRI data of 1,498 adults participating in a picture encoding task in a single MRI scanner. We find that individual differences in responsivity of the hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex account for individual variability in episodic memory performance. While these regions also emerge in our group-level analysis, other regions, predominantly within the lateral occipital cortex, are related to successful memory encoding but not to individual memory variation. Furthermore, our network-based approach reveals a link between the responsivity of nine functional connectivity networks and individual memory variability. Our work provides insights into the neurofunctional correlates of individual differences in visual episodic memory performance.
<<<
翻译
16.
muton
(2023-08-31 23:17):
#paper Multiple routes to enhanced memory for emotionally relevant events https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2023.06.006令人厌恶的负性事件或奖赏有关的正性事件会被记得更好。这种记忆力的增强通常是因为引发了情感反应,这一过程与去甲肾上腺素和多巴胺调节的海马可塑性密切相关。最新发现表明预期偏差是上述事件会被记得更好的原因。在“预测”机制中,记忆会随着结果偏离预期的程度(即预测误差(PE)而得到加强)。 PE 对记忆的影响与情感结果本身是分开的,并且具有独特的神经特征。虽然这两种途径都能增强记忆,但两种机制会预测不同(有时甚至是相反)记忆整合的结果。文章讨论的一些新的研究结果强调了情绪事件增强、整合和分割记忆的机制。
Abstract:
Events associated with aversive or rewarding outcomes are prioritized in memory. This memory boost is commonly attributed to the elicited affective response, closely linked to noradrenergic and dopaminergic modulation of …
>>>
Events associated with aversive or rewarding outcomes are prioritized in memory. This memory boost is commonly attributed to the elicited affective response, closely linked to noradrenergic and dopaminergic modulation of hippocampal plasticity. Herein we review and compare this 'affect' mechanism to an additional, recently discovered, 'prediction' mechanism whereby memories are strengthened by the extent to which outcomes deviate from expectations, that is, by prediction errors (PEs). The mnemonic impact of PEs is separate from the affective outcome itself and has a distinct neural signature. While both routes enhance memory, these mechanisms are linked to different - and sometimes opposing - predictions for memory integration. We discuss new findings that highlight mechanisms by which emotional events strengthen, integrate, and segment memory.
<<<
翻译
17.
muton
(2023-07-31 22:52):
#paper:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.09.044 The memory trace of a stressful episode压力通过去甲肾上腺素和糖皮质激素对杏仁核和海马体的影响来影响情景记忆的形成。以往研究发现,当我们处在压力情境中时会有一种聚焦效应,也就是对情景中心方面的事情会记得更好。但是如何来用神经证据证明这一观点还未可知。本文作者使用神经表征相似性分析的方法分析了杏仁核脑区在压力情景下遇到物体的记忆情况。结果发现,出现在中心的物体比起出现在两边的物体神经表征的相似性会更高。并且这种更高的相似性预测了更好的记忆效果。这表明,中心物体被紧密地整合到一个以压力为中心的记忆表征中。本研究运用了一种生活化的实验范式巧妙设计并分析了压力情景下的“记忆痕迹”。
Abstract:
Stress influences episodic memory formation via noradrenaline and glucocorticoid effects on amygdala and hippocampus. A common finding is the improvement of memory for central aspects of a stressful episode. This …
>>>
Stress influences episodic memory formation via noradrenaline and glucocorticoid effects on amygdala and hippocampus. A common finding is the improvement of memory for central aspects of a stressful episode. This is putatively related to changes in the neural representations of specific experiences, i.e., their memory traces. Here we show that the memory improvement for objects that were encountered in a stressful episode relates to differences in the neural representations of these objects in the amygdala. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we found that stress specifically altered the representations of central objects: compared to control objects, they became more similar to one another and more distinct from objects that were not part of this episode. Furthermore, higher similarity of central objects to the main stressor-the faces of the stress-inducing committee members-predicted better memory. This suggests that the central objects were closely integrated into a stressor-centered memory representation. Our findings provide mechanistic insights into how stress shapes the memory trace and have profound implications for neurocognitive models of stressful and emotional memory.
<<<
翻译
18.
muton
(2023-06-30 21:57):
# Paper: Doeller, C. F., Barry, C., & Burgess, N. (2010). Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature, 463(7281), 657–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08704
自由运动大鼠内嗅皮层中的网格细胞提供了自我定位的显著周期性表征,这表明了非常特殊的计算机制。然而,网格细胞在人类中的存在及其在整个大脑中的分布是未知的。本研究表明,大鼠内嗅皮层定向调节的网格细胞的优先放电方向与网格对齐,并且快跑与慢跑相比,网格细胞放电的空间组织更为明显。由于网格细胞的一致性特征,作者预测了在更宏观的信号,也就是人类的功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)记录中也会发现网格细胞的相关证据。因此,作者模拟老鼠的觅食任务,让参与者探索虚拟现实环境,结果显示fMRI激活和适应在跑步方向上显示出由速度调节的六倍旋转对称的特性。这种信号在鼻内/丘下、顶叶后部和内侧、颞外侧和前额叶内侧的网络中都有发现。并且该效应在右内嗅皮层表现最为明显,方向信号在内嗅皮层间的相干性与空间记忆表现相关。本研究提示了将单细胞电生理学与功能磁共振成像在系统神经科学中结合的潜在可能。本研究结果为人类的网格细胞样表征提供了证据,并暗示在支持空间认知和自传体记忆的区域网络中存在一种特定类型神经表征。
Abstract:
Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats provide a strikingly periodic representation of self-location which is indicative of very specific computational mechanisms. However, the existence of grid …
>>>
Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats provide a strikingly periodic representation of self-location which is indicative of very specific computational mechanisms. However, the existence of grid cells in humans and their distribution throughout the brain are unknown. Here we show that the preferred firing directions of directionally modulated grid cells in rat entorhinal cortex are aligned with the grids, and that the spatial organization of grid-cell firing is more strongly apparent at faster than slower running speeds. Because the grids are also aligned with each other, we predicted a macroscopic signal visible to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. We then looked for this signal as participants explored a virtual reality environment, mimicking the rats' foraging task: fMRI activation and adaptation showing a speed-modulated six-fold rotational symmetry in running direction. The signal was found in a network of entorhinal/subicular, posterior and medial parietal, lateral temporal and medial prefrontal areas. The effect was strongest in right entorhinal cortex, and the coherence of the directional signal across entorhinal cortex correlated with spatial memory performance. Our study illustrates the potential power of combining single-unit electrophysiology with fMRI in systems neuroscience. Our results provide evidence for grid-cell-like representations in humans, and implicate a specific type of neural representation in a network of regions which supports spatial cognition and also autobiographical memory.
<<<
翻译
19.
muton
(2023-05-31 22:39):
# paper:Challenging the Classical View: Recognition of Identity and Expression as Integrated Processes. https://doi.org/10.3390/ brainsci13020296 最近神经影像学的证据挑战了以往关于人脸信息特征和面部表情由不同神经通路分别加工处理的经典观点,而是认为身份和表情的信息在共同的脑区被编码。作者基于这一背景利用深度卷积神经网络分别对面孔身份和面孔表情的数据集进行了训练,结果发现各自训练后的神经网络不仅可以分别很好的解码身份/表情,同时对于解码未训练过的表情/身份时也有较好的表现。这一结果验证了上述假设。
Abstract:
Recent neuroimaging evidence challenges the classical view that face identity and facial expression are processed by segregated neural pathways, showing that information about identity and expression are encoded within common …
>>>
Recent neuroimaging evidence challenges the classical view that face identity and facial expression are processed by segregated neural pathways, showing that information about identity and expression are encoded within common brain regions. This article tests the hypothesis that integrated representations of identity and expression arise spontaneously within deep neural networks. A subset of the CelebA dataset is used to train a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN) to label face identity (chance = 0.06%, accuracy = 26.5%), and the FER2013 dataset is used to train a DCNN to label facial expression (chance = 14.2%, accuracy = 63.5%). The identity-trained and expression-trained networks each successfully transfer to labeling both face identity and facial expression on the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces dataset. This study demonstrates that DCNNs trained to recognize face identity and DCNNs trained to recognize facial expression spontaneously develop representations of facial expression and face identity, respectively. Furthermore, a congruence coefficient analysis reveals that features distinguishing between identities and features distinguishing between expressions become increasingly orthogonal from layer to layer, suggesting that deep neural networks disentangle representational subspaces corresponding to different sources.
<<<
翻译
20.
muton
(2023-04-30 23:19):
#paper Amygdala and cortical gamma-band responses to emotional faces depend on the attended to valence https://arxiv.org/pdf/2304.05700.pdf 杏仁核被认为贡献于情绪面孔视觉加工中自下而上的注意偏好,然而其对于情绪的反应如何与自上而下的注意相互作用却并不清楚。并且,杏仁核对情绪和注意的反应与头皮脑电相比有多大程度相似也仍有待探究。因此作者分别记录了杏仁核脑区的颅内电极以及头皮脑电伽马段的脑电活动来探究面孔加工过程中情绪和注意的交互。结果发现,在情绪检测实验中杏仁核的高频伽马出现在以中性面孔作为识别目标时,当以负性面孔作为识别目标时,低频伽马在负性面孔出现时会显著增加,并且不仅局限于杏仁核,同时在后部脑区头皮脑电记录中也存在,且时间窗早于杏仁核。这一结果符合情绪加工的多通路模型,并且是从注意(自上而下)的角度发现了伽马波在加工情绪面孔中的作用。
arXiv,
2023.
DOI: 10.48550/arXiv.2304.05700
Abstract:
The amygdala is assumed to contribute to a bottom-up attentional bias during visual processing of emotional faces. Still, how its response to emotion interacts with top-down attention is not fully …
>>>
The amygdala is assumed to contribute to a bottom-up attentional bias during visual processing of emotional faces. Still, how its response to emotion interacts with top-down attention is not fully understood. It is also unclear if amygdala activity and scalp EEG respond to emotion and attention in a similar way. Therefore, we studied the interaction of emotion and attention during face processing in oscillatory gamma-band activity (GBA) in the amygdala and on the scalp. Amygdala signals were recorded via intracranial EEG (iEEG) in 9 patients with epilepsy. Scalp recordings were collected from 19 healthy participants. Three randomized blocks of angry, neutral, and happy faces were presented, and either negative, neutral, or positive expressions were denoted as targets. Both groups detected happy faces fastest and most accurately. In the amygdala, the earliest effect was observed around 170 ms in high GBA (105-117.5 Hz) when neutral faces served as targets. Here, GBA was higher for emotional than neutral faces. During attention to negative faces, low GBA (< 90 Hz) increased specifically for angry faces both in the amygdala and over posterior scalp regions, albeit earlier on the scalp (60 ms) than in the amygdala (210 ms). From 570 ms, amygdala high GBA (117.5-145 Hz) was also increased for both angry and neutral, compared to happy, faces. When positive faces were the targets, GBA did not differentiate between expressions. The present data reveal that attention-independent emotion detection in amygdala high GBA may only occur during a neutral focus of attention. Top-down threat vigilance coordinates widespread low GBA, biasing stimulus processing in favor of negative faces. These results are in line with a multi-pathway model of emotion processing and help specify the role of GBA in this process by revealing how attentional focus can tune timing and amplitude of emotional GBA responses.
<<<
翻译