来自用户 muton 的文献。
当前共找到 32 篇文献分享,本页显示第 21 - 32 篇。
21.
muton (2023-06-30 21:57):
# Paper: Doeller, C. F., Barry, C., & Burgess, N. (2010). Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature, 463(7281), 657–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08704 自由运动大鼠内嗅皮层中的网格细胞提供了自我定位的显著周期性表征,这表明了非常特殊的计算机制。然而,网格细胞在人类中的存在及其在整个大脑中的分布是未知的。本研究表明,大鼠内嗅皮层定向调节的网格细胞的优先放电方向与网格对齐,并且快跑与慢跑相比,网格细胞放电的空间组织更为明显。由于网格细胞的一致性特征,作者预测了在更宏观的信号,也就是人类的功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)记录中也会发现网格细胞的相关证据。因此,作者模拟老鼠的觅食任务,让参与者探索虚拟现实环境,结果显示fMRI激活和适应在跑步方向上显示出由速度调节的六倍旋转对称的特性。这种信号在鼻内/丘下、顶叶后部和内侧、颞外侧和前额叶内侧的网络中都有发现。并且该效应在右内嗅皮层表现最为明显,方向信号在内嗅皮层间的相干性与空间记忆表现相关。本研究提示了将单细胞电生理学与功能磁共振成像在系统神经科学中结合的潜在可能。本研究结果为人类的网格细胞样表征提供了证据,并暗示在支持空间认知和自传体记忆的区域网络中存在一种特定类型神经表征。
IF:50.500Q1 Nature, 2010-Feb-04. DOI: 10.1038/nature08704 PMID: 20090680
Abstract:
Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats provide a strikingly periodic representation of self-location which is indicative of very specific computational mechanisms. However, the existence of grid … >>>
Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats provide a strikingly periodic representation of self-location which is indicative of very specific computational mechanisms. However, the existence of grid cells in humans and their distribution throughout the brain are unknown. Here we show that the preferred firing directions of directionally modulated grid cells in rat entorhinal cortex are aligned with the grids, and that the spatial organization of grid-cell firing is more strongly apparent at faster than slower running speeds. Because the grids are also aligned with each other, we predicted a macroscopic signal visible to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. We then looked for this signal as participants explored a virtual reality environment, mimicking the rats' foraging task: fMRI activation and adaptation showing a speed-modulated six-fold rotational symmetry in running direction. The signal was found in a network of entorhinal/subicular, posterior and medial parietal, lateral temporal and medial prefrontal areas. The effect was strongest in right entorhinal cortex, and the coherence of the directional signal across entorhinal cortex correlated with spatial memory performance. Our study illustrates the potential power of combining single-unit electrophysiology with fMRI in systems neuroscience. Our results provide evidence for grid-cell-like representations in humans, and implicate a specific type of neural representation in a network of regions which supports spatial cognition and also autobiographical memory. <<<
翻译
22.
muton (2023-05-31 22:39):
# paper:Challenging the Classical View: Recognition of Identity and Expression as Integrated Processes. https://doi.org/10.3390/ brainsci13020296 最近神经影像学的证据挑战了以往关于人脸信息特征和面部表情由不同神经通路分别加工处理的经典观点,而是认为身份和表情的信息在共同的脑区被编码。作者基于这一背景利用深度卷积神经网络分别对面孔身份和面孔表情的数据集进行了训练,结果发现各自训练后的神经网络不仅可以分别很好的解码身份/表情,同时对于解码未训练过的表情/身份时也有较好的表现。这一结果验证了上述假设。
IF:2.700Q3 Brain sciences, 2023-Feb-10. DOI: 10.3390/brainsci13020296 PMID: 36831839
Abstract:
Recent neuroimaging evidence challenges the classical view that face identity and facial expression are processed by segregated neural pathways, showing that information about identity and expression are encoded within common … >>>
Recent neuroimaging evidence challenges the classical view that face identity and facial expression are processed by segregated neural pathways, showing that information about identity and expression are encoded within common brain regions. This article tests the hypothesis that integrated representations of identity and expression arise spontaneously within deep neural networks. A subset of the CelebA dataset is used to train a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN) to label face identity (chance = 0.06%, accuracy = 26.5%), and the FER2013 dataset is used to train a DCNN to label facial expression (chance = 14.2%, accuracy = 63.5%). The identity-trained and expression-trained networks each successfully transfer to labeling both face identity and facial expression on the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces dataset. This study demonstrates that DCNNs trained to recognize face identity and DCNNs trained to recognize facial expression spontaneously develop representations of facial expression and face identity, respectively. Furthermore, a congruence coefficient analysis reveals that features distinguishing between identities and features distinguishing between expressions become increasingly orthogonal from layer to layer, suggesting that deep neural networks disentangle representational subspaces corresponding to different sources. <<<
翻译
23.
muton (2023-04-30 23:19):
#paper Amygdala and cortical gamma-band responses to emotional faces depend on the attended to valence https://arxiv.org/pdf/2304.05700.pdf 杏仁核被认为贡献于情绪面孔视觉加工中自下而上的注意偏好,然而其对于情绪的反应如何与自上而下的注意相互作用却并不清楚。并且,杏仁核对情绪和注意的反应与头皮脑电相比有多大程度相似也仍有待探究。因此作者分别记录了杏仁核脑区的颅内电极以及头皮脑电伽马段的脑电活动来探究面孔加工过程中情绪和注意的交互。结果发现,在情绪检测实验中杏仁核的高频伽马出现在以中性面孔作为识别目标时,当以负性面孔作为识别目标时,低频伽马在负性面孔出现时会显著增加,并且不仅局限于杏仁核,同时在后部脑区头皮脑电记录中也存在,且时间窗早于杏仁核。这一结果符合情绪加工的多通路模型,并且是从注意(自上而下)的角度发现了伽马波在加工情绪面孔中的作用。
Abstract:
The amygdala is assumed to contribute to a bottom-up attentional bias during visual processing of emotional faces. Still, how its response to emotion interacts with top-down attention is not fully … >>>
The amygdala is assumed to contribute to a bottom-up attentional bias during visual processing of emotional faces. Still, how its response to emotion interacts with top-down attention is not fully understood. It is also unclear if amygdala activity and scalp EEG respond to emotion and attention in a similar way. Therefore, we studied the interaction of emotion and attention during face processing in oscillatory gamma-band activity (GBA) in the amygdala and on the scalp. Amygdala signals were recorded via intracranial EEG (iEEG) in 9 patients with epilepsy. Scalp recordings were collected from 19 healthy participants. Three randomized blocks of angry, neutral, and happy faces were presented, and either negative, neutral, or positive expressions were denoted as targets. Both groups detected happy faces fastest and most accurately. In the amygdala, the earliest effect was observed around 170 ms in high GBA (105-117.5 Hz) when neutral faces served as targets. Here, GBA was higher for emotional than neutral faces. During attention to negative faces, low GBA (< 90 Hz) increased specifically for angry faces both in the amygdala and over posterior scalp regions, albeit earlier on the scalp (60 ms) than in the amygdala (210 ms). From 570 ms, amygdala high GBA (117.5-145 Hz) was also increased for both angry and neutral, compared to happy, faces. When positive faces were the targets, GBA did not differentiate between expressions. The present data reveal that attention-independent emotion detection in amygdala high GBA may only occur during a neutral focus of attention. Top-down threat vigilance coordinates widespread low GBA, biasing stimulus processing in favor of negative faces. These results are in line with a multi-pathway model of emotion processing and help specify the role of GBA in this process by revealing how attentional focus can tune timing and amplitude of emotional GBA responses. <<<
翻译
24.
muton (2023-03-31 22:44):
#paper https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.82250 Extra-hippocampal contributions to pattern separation 模式分离,或记忆中高度相似的刺激或经验由不重叠的神经组合代表的过程,通常被归结为由海马体支持的过程。然而,广泛研究的证据表明,模式分离是一个由大脑区域网络支持的多阶段过程。基于这些证据,再加上文献中的相关发现,作者提出了 "皮质-海马模式分离"(CHiPS)框架,认为参与认知控制的大脑区域在模式分离中发挥了重要作用。特别是,这些区域可能通过以下方式对模式分离做出贡献:(1)解决投射到海马的感觉区域的干扰,从而调节其皮质输入;或(2)根据任务要求直接调节海马的过程。本文创新之处是作者认为模式分离也同样受到新皮层-海马而不仅仅是海马的支持。
IF:6.400Q1 eLife, 2023-03-27. DOI: 10.7554/eLife.82250 PMID: 36972123
Abstract:
Pattern separation, or the process by which highly similar stimuli or experiences in memory are represented by non-overlapping neural ensembles, has typically been ascribed to processes supported by the hippocampus. … >>>
Pattern separation, or the process by which highly similar stimuli or experiences in memory are represented by non-overlapping neural ensembles, has typically been ascribed to processes supported by the hippocampus. Converging evidence from a wide range of studies, however, suggests that pattern separation is a multistage process supported by a network of brain regions. Based on this evidence, considered together with related findings from the interference resolution literature, we propose the 'cortico-hippocampal pattern separation' (CHiPS) framework, which asserts that brain regions involved in cognitive control play a significant role in pattern separation. Particularly, these regions may contribute to pattern separation by (1) resolving interference in sensory regions that project to the hippocampus, thus regulating its cortical input, or (2) directly modulating hippocampal processes in accordance with task demands. Considering recent interest in how hippocampal operations are modulated by goal states likely represented and regulated by extra-hippocampal regions, we argue that pattern separation is similarly supported by neocortical-hippocampal interactions. <<<
翻译
25.
muton (2023-02-28 22:04):
#paper DOI: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-2540186/v1 Sleep loss diminishes hippocampal reactivation and replay睡眠有助于记忆,如果学习后立即剥夺睡眠对随后的记忆储存有负面影响。一些著名的假说认为,在睡眠期间发生的离线记忆巩固过程中,海马的尖波涟漪(SWRs)以及清醒时的神经元模式的同时重新激活和记忆重放起着核心作用。然而,当动物被剥夺睡眠时,SWRs、重新激活和重放是如何被影响的,人们对此所知甚少。作者对大鼠海马CA1神经元进行了长时间(约12小时)、高密度的硅探针记录,在这些动物在暴露于一个新的迷宫环境后,一些进入睡眠状态,一些被剥夺睡眠。我们发现,在睡眠剥夺期间,SWRs显示出持续的活动率,类似于或高于自然睡眠,但尖锐波的振幅下降,同时波纹的频率更高。此外,虽然海马锥体细胞在睡眠期间显示出对数正态分布的放电频率,但这些分布是负偏斜的,在睡眠剥夺期间,锥体细胞和中间神经元的平均放电频率都较高。然而,在SWRs期间,两组的放电频率非常相似。尽管两组都有大量的SWRs,并有强烈的放电活动,但我们发现,与睡眠剥夺相比,神经元的重新激活在睡眠剥夺期间要么完全没有,要么明显减少。有趣的是,在恢复睡眠后,重新激活部分又出现,但未能达到自然睡眠的特征水平。同样,与自然睡眠相比,在睡眠剥夺和恢复性睡眠期间,重放的数量明显减少。这些结果为睡眠损失对海马功能的负面影响提供了一个网络层面的解释,并证明睡眠损失通过导致SWRs的数量与这些事件中发生的记忆重放和重新激活之间的分离来影响记忆储存。
Abstract:
Memories benefit from sleep, and sleep loss immediately following learning has a negative impact on subsequent memory storage. Several prominent hypotheses ascribe a central role to hippocampal sharp-wave ripples (SWRs), … >>>
Memories benefit from sleep, and sleep loss immediately following learning has a negative impact on subsequent memory storage. Several prominent hypotheses ascribe a central role to hippocampal sharp-wave ripples (SWRs), and the concurrent reactivation and replay of neuronal patterns from waking experience, in the offline memory consolidation process that occurs during sleep. However, little is known about how SWRs, reactivation, and replay are affected when animals are subjected to sleep deprivation. We performed long duration (~12 h), high-density silicon probe recordings from rat hippocampal CA1 neurons, in animals that were either sleeping or sleep deprived following exposure to a novel maze environment. We found that SWRs showed a sustained rate of activity during sleep deprivation, similar to or higher than in natural sleep, but with decreased amplitudes for the sharp-waves combined with higher frequencies for the ripples. Furthermore, while hippocampal pyramidal cells showed a log-normal distribution of firing rates during sleep, these distributions were negatively skewed with a higher mean firing rate in both pyramidal cells and interneurons during sleep deprivation. During SWRs, however, firing rates were remarkably similar between both groups. Despite the abundant quantity of SWRs and the robust firing activity during these events in both groups, we found that reactivation of neurons was either completely abolished or significantly diminished during sleep deprivation compared to sleep. Interestingly, reactivation partially rebounded upon recovery sleep, but failed to reach the levels characteristic of natural sleep. Similarly, the number of replays were significantly lower during sleep deprivation and recovery sleep compared to natural sleep. These results provide a network-level account for the negative impact of sleep loss on hippocampal function and demonstrate that sleep loss impacts memory storage by causing a dissociation between the amount of SWRs and the replays and reactivations that take place during these events. <<<
翻译
26.
muton (2023-01-31 23:03):
#paper # Yu, W., Zadbood, A., Chanales, A. J., & Davachi, L. (2022). Repetition accelerates neural markers of memory consolidation. bioRxiv, 2022-12.https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.12.14.520481; 认知加工过程中一旦体验结束,神经记忆表征就开始通过记忆回放的过程得到加强和转化。使用功能磁共振成像技术,作者研究了编码过程中通过重复操纵而改变的记忆强度如何调节人类的编码后回放。结果显示,重复不能增强海马的回放频率,但是皮层区域的回放以及皮层海马共同协调的回放在重复事件中被显著增强,表明重复加速了记忆巩固的过程,另外在海马和皮层的回放频率可以调节即时联想辨认测试中编码较弱的信息的行为成功率,这表明了编码后回放在帮助回忆曾经出现过事件的重要作用。总的来说这篇文章突出了回放在巩固较弱记忆和加速皮层记忆巩固来增强记忆过程中的作用。
Abstract:
AbstractNo sooner is an experience over than its neural memory representation begins to be strengthened and transformed through the process of memory replay. Using fMRI, we examined how memory strength … >>>
AbstractNo sooner is an experience over than its neural memory representation begins to be strengthened and transformed through the process of memory replay. Using fMRI, we examined how memory strength manipulated through repetition during encoding modulates post-encoding replay in humans. Results revealed that repetition did not increase replay frequency in the hippocampus. However, replay in cortical regions and hippocampal-cortical coordinated replay were significantly enhanced for repeated events, suggesting that repetition accelerates the consolidation process. Interestingly, we found that replay frequency in both hippocampus and cortex modulated behavioral success on an immediate associative recognition test for the weakly encoded information, indicating a significant role for post-encoding replay in rescuing once-presented events. Together, our findings highlight the relationships of replay to stabilizing weak memories and accelerating cortical consolidation for strong memories. <<<
翻译
27.
muton (2022-12-31 22:43):
#paper doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.10.03.510672 Human hippocampal ripples signal encoding of episodic memories biorixv 2022 海马尖波涟漪是在哺乳动物电生理中发现的一个很特别具有代表性的成分,最开始是在小鼠研究中被发现,随着人类脑电记录的发展,颅内记录的出现让研究尖波涟漪在人类中变为现实,以往在人类的研究中更多关注于ripple和记忆提取之间的关系,很少研究在编码信息,尤其是单个项目时ripple的作用,本文则填补了这一空白,通过124名被试的情景记忆任务表现,作者发现虽然在MTL等重要脑区能够发现高频信号的随后记忆效应,但ripple并未表现出差异,但令人新奇的是ripple会在记忆item在编码时间上相近或语义相近的item时表现出更频繁的发放,也被称为一种聚类效应,并且这一现象在编码和提取阶段都能够被发现,这种现象可能代表了一种对于记忆的保留,有助于预测和提取记忆。本篇文章对于探究ripple这一脑电成分在人类情景记忆中的功能有重要提示。
Abstract:
AbstractRecent human electrophysiology work has uncovered the presence of high frequency oscillatory events, termed ripples, during awake behavior. This prior work focuses on ripples in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) … >>>
AbstractRecent human electrophysiology work has uncovered the presence of high frequency oscillatory events, termed ripples, during awake behavior. This prior work focuses on ripples in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) during memory retrieval. Few studies, however, investigate ripples during item encoding. Many studies have found neural activity during encoding that predicts later recall, termed subsequent memory effects (SMEs), but it is unclear if ripples during encoding also predict subsequent recall. Detecting ripples in 124 neurosurgical participants performing an episodic memory task, we find insignificant ripple SMEs in any MTL region, even as these regions exhibit robust high frequency activity (HFA) SMEs. Instead, hippocampal ripples increase during encoding of items leading to recall of temporally or semantically associated items, a phenomenon known as clustering. This subsequent clustering effect (SCE) arises specifically when hippocampal ripples occur during both encoding and retrieval, suggesting that ripples mediate the encoding and future reinstatement of episodic memories. <<<
翻译
28.
muton (2022-11-30 23:19):
#paper https://science.org/doi/10.1126/ sciadv.abm3829 Science Advances,2022,Higher-dimensional neural representations predict better episodic memory 情景记忆使人类能够编码并随后生动地检索有关我们丰富经历的信息,但怎样的神经表征可以支持这一心理能力?作者让被试学习人脸图片和词语的配对,使用表征维度的分析方法,对由脑成像得到的神经相似性矩阵进行PCA分析,得到每个主成分的eigenvalue,通过对eigenvalue的处理得到RD(representational dimensionality)值,来分析面孔选择区和其他相关脑区的差异,结果发现,面孔选择区保留了高维表征,重要的是,RD值越大,记忆效应就越好。本文提供了新的神经表征分析方法。
IF:11.700Q1 Science advances, 2022-Apr-22. DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abm3829 PMID: 35442734
Abstract:
Episodic memory enables humans to encode and later vividly retrieve information about our rich experiences, yet the neural representations that support this mental capacity are poorly understood. Using a large … >>>
Episodic memory enables humans to encode and later vividly retrieve information about our rich experiences, yet the neural representations that support this mental capacity are poorly understood. Using a large fMRI dataset ( = 468) of face-name associative memory tasks and principal component analysis to examine neural representational dimensionality (RD), we found that the human brain maintained a high-dimensional representation of faces through hierarchical representation within and beyond the face-selective regions. Critically, greater RD was associated with better subsequent memory performance both within and across participants, and this association was specific to episodic memory but not general cognitive abilities. Furthermore, the frontoparietal activities could suppress the shared low-dimensional fluctuations and reduce the correlations of local neural responses, resulting in greater RD. RD was not associated with the degree of item-specific pattern similarity, and it made complementary contributions to episodic memory. These results provide a mechanistic understanding of the role of RD in supporting accurate episodic memory. <<<
翻译
29.
muton (2022-10-31 23:23):
#paper https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1449-20.2020 FFA and OFA Encode Distinct Types of Face Identity Information 在人的大脑中有诸多加工人脸的脑区,然而,这些加工人脸的脑区各自负责加工人脸当中的什么信息呢?作者基于此点思考,对三个面孔识别区(FFA梭状回、OFA枕叶面孔加工区和pSTS颞上沟)观看不同人脸得到的fMRI数据使用表征相似性分析的方法进行分析,比较了每个脑区的表征距离和代表身份信息模型的差异,结果发现FFA和OFA所代表的信息存在显著差异。FFA中人脸身份之间的差异是由感知相似性,社会特征,性别和OpenFace网络的差异引起的。相比之下,OFA中的表征距离主要是由低级的基于图像属性的差异(像素级和Gabor-Jet差异)驱动的。也就是说,尽管FFA和OFA都可以区分身份,但FFA相比于OFA,编码更高层次的感知和社会面孔的信息。
Abstract:
Faces of different people elicit distinct fMRI patterns in several face-selective regions of the human brain. Here we used representational similarity analysis to investigate what type of identity-distinguishing information is … >>>
Faces of different people elicit distinct fMRI patterns in several face-selective regions of the human brain. Here we used representational similarity analysis to investigate what type of identity-distinguishing information is encoded in three face-selective regions: fusiform face area (FFA), occipital face area (OFA), and posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). In a sample of 30 human participants (22 females, 8 males), we used fMRI to measure brain activity patterns elicited by naturalistic videos of famous face identities, and compared their representational distances in each region with models of the differences between identities. We built diverse candidate models, ranging from low-level image-computable properties (pixel-wise, GIST, and Gabor-Jet dissimilarities), through higher-level image-computable descriptions (OpenFace deep neural network, trained to cluster faces by identity), to complex human-rated properties (perceived similarity, social traits, and gender). We found marked differences in the information represented by the FFA and OFA. Dissimilarities between face identities in FFA were accounted for by differences in perceived similarity, Social Traits, Gender, and by the OpenFace network. In contrast, representational distances in OFA were mainly driven by differences in low-level image-based properties (pixel-wise and Gabor-Jet dissimilarities). Our results suggest that, although FFA and OFA can both discriminate between identities, the FFA representation is further removed from the image, encoding higher-level perceptual and social face information. Recent studies using fMRI have shown that several face-responsive brain regions can distinguish between different face identities. It is however unclear whether these different face-responsive regions distinguish between identities in similar or different ways. We used representational similarity analysis to investigate the computations within three brain regions in response to naturalistically varying videos of face identities. Our results revealed that two regions, the fusiform face area and the occipital face area, encode distinct identity information about faces. Although identity can be decoded from both regions, identity representations in fusiform face area primarily contained information about social traits, gender, and high-level visual features, whereas occipital face area primarily represented lower-level image features. <<<
翻译
30.
muton (2022-09-30 21:34):
#paper doi:10.1038/nature02223 Sleep inspires insight. 睡眠激发洞察力(Insight):洞察力表示一种心理重组的过程,指突然获得显性知识,从而使行为发生质的变化的过程。科学报告表明,可以通过睡眠获得这种洞察力(有点像顿悟的意思)。睡眠可以巩固最近的记忆,同时,可以通过改变其表征结构来获得洞察力。本文中,作者让被试执行了一种需要学习刺激-反应序列的认知任务,在学习这项任务过程中,被试通过提高任务组块间的反应速度体现出了表现的提升。然而,被试也可以通过洞察到所有序列背后隐藏的抽象规则来提高行为表现。实验条件分为三种:初始训练后的 8 小时夜间睡眠、夜间清醒或白天清醒。在随后的重新测试中,睡眠后对隐藏规则的洞察力是清醒后的两倍多。然而在没有初始训练的情况下,睡眠并不能增强洞察力,所以,本文认为睡眠通过重构新的记忆表征,有助于提取显性知识和有洞察力的行为。行为实验例子:让被试在给定的两个规则中将8位数字序列减少到7位,这两个规则是相同规则:如两个相同的数字仍得到此数字;还有不同规则:即两个数字不同得到另外一个数字:如11449494,通过前两位得到1,然后1和4比较得到9,最终得到1914419的7位数字序列,然而没有告诉被试的是在最终得到的7位数字序列中,2-4位和5-7位总是镜像对称的,如果被试能够发现这一隐藏规则,其反应时会大大减少,而作者发现在之前睡眠或清醒的三个条件中,在经过睡眠之后,能够发现这一隐藏规则的被试大大增加,也表明了睡眠对于获得洞察力的贡献。
IF:50.500Q1 Nature, 2004-Jan-22. PMID: 14737168
Abstract:
Insight denotes a mental restructuring that leads to a sudden gain of explicit knowledge allowing qualitatively changed behaviour. Anecdotal reports on scientific discovery suggest that pivotal insights can be gained … >>>
Insight denotes a mental restructuring that leads to a sudden gain of explicit knowledge allowing qualitatively changed behaviour. Anecdotal reports on scientific discovery suggest that pivotal insights can be gained through sleep. Sleep consolidates recent memories and, concomitantly, could allow insight by changing their representational structure. Here we show a facilitating role of sleep in a process of insight. Subjects performed a cognitive task requiring the learning of stimulus-response sequences, in which they improved gradually by increasing response speed across task blocks. However, they could also improve abruptly after gaining insight into a hidden abstract rule underlying all sequences. Initial training establishing a task representation was followed by 8 h of nocturnal sleep, nocturnal wakefulness, or daytime wakefulness. At subsequent retesting, more than twice as many subjects gained insight into the hidden rule after sleep as after wakefulness, regardless of time of day. Sleep did not enhance insight in the absence of initial training. A characteristic antecedent of sleep-related insight was revealed in a slowing of reaction times across sleep. We conclude that sleep, by restructuring new memory representations, facilitates extraction of explicit knowledge and insightful behaviour. <<<
翻译
31.
muton (2022-08-31 21:16):
#paper DOI: 10.1038/nn.4324 A fast pathway for fear in human amygdala 关于杏仁核的多类研究证明其在恐惧中发挥的作用。有一种能够快速到达杏仁核的皮层下通路被认为是为了能够快速检测威胁而进化出来的。这一通路的存在是理解无意识情绪反应的基础,但由于缺乏包括人类在内的灵长类动物杏仁核中短潜伏期恐惧相关反应的证据,如何验证这一通路是一个困难的问题。本文作者记录了人类颅内电生理数据,发现了在刺激开始后的74毫秒,杏仁核对恐惧而不是中性或快乐的面部表情的快速反应。这些反应的潜伏期比我们在视觉皮层观察到的恐惧反应要短得多。值得注意的是,杏仁核的快速反应仅限于恐惧面孔的低空间频率成分。此外,快速的杏仁核反应不会被场景图片唤起,这表明,人们对恐惧的面孔传达的与社会相关的视觉信息有选择性的早期反应。总而言之,本文数据支持了一种系统发育历史悠久的皮层下通路的存在,该通路向人类杏仁核提供快速但粗糙的威胁相关信号。
IF:21.200Q1 Nature neuroscience, 2016-08. DOI: 10.1038/nn.4324 PMID: 27294508
Abstract:
A fast, subcortical pathway to the amygdala is thought to have evolved to enable rapid detection of threat. This pathway's existence is fundamental for understanding nonconscious emotional responses, but has … >>>
A fast, subcortical pathway to the amygdala is thought to have evolved to enable rapid detection of threat. This pathway's existence is fundamental for understanding nonconscious emotional responses, but has been challenged as a result of a lack of evidence for short-latency fear-related responses in primate amygdala, including humans. We recorded human intracranial electrophysiological data and found fast amygdala responses, beginning 74-ms post-stimulus onset, to fearful, but not neutral or happy, facial expressions. These responses had considerably shorter latency than fear responses that we observed in visual cortex. Notably, fast amygdala responses were limited to low spatial frequency components of fearful faces, as predicted by magnocellular inputs to amygdala. Furthermore, fast amygdala responses were not evoked by photographs of arousing scenes, which is indicative of selective early reactivity to socially relevant visual information conveyed by fearful faces. These data therefore support the existence of a phylogenetically old subcortical pathway providing fast, but coarse, threat-related signals to human amygdala. <<<
翻译
32.
muton (2022-07-28 11:58):
#paper DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009267 Unveiling functions of the visual cortex using task-specific deep neural networks.人类的视觉感知是一种复杂的认知能力,它是由大脑不同皮层区域控制调节的。然而目前这些区域的确切功能我们了解的仍不完全清楚,进而这些区域是如何协调视觉感知的也没有确切的答案。目前的观点认为视觉信息的转变过程是通过不同功能区域的层次化计算,通常我们概括为这些功能区域为腹侧和背侧视觉通路。无论是发现各个视觉皮层区域的确切功能还是利用计算建模的方法实现这种功能都是具有挑战性的,但也是我们的最终诉求。深度神经网络(DNNs)用于实现建模和预测视觉区域反应的一种较有前景的方法。本文通过比较不同视觉任务中的fMRI数据集与针对不同视觉任务优化过的DNN 模型子集的相关(作者选择了通过Taskonomy数据集训练的18个DNNs模型,这些模型分别对应于室内场景图片理解的18个不同任务的优化)发现了视觉信息沿腹侧和背侧视觉通路的结构化映射。低级视觉任务映射到早期视觉皮层,三维场景感知任务映射到背侧流,语义任务映射到腹侧流。文章的亮点可能就是通过模型和人脑实际数据相似性比较的方法能够得出哪些脑区贡献于哪些任务的这种思路。
Abstract:
The human visual cortex enables visual perception through a cascade of hierarchical computations in cortical regions with distinct functionalities. Here, we introduce an AI-driven approach to discover the functional mapping … >>>
The human visual cortex enables visual perception through a cascade of hierarchical computations in cortical regions with distinct functionalities. Here, we introduce an AI-driven approach to discover the functional mapping of the visual cortex. We related human brain responses to scene images measured with functional MRI (fMRI) systematically to a diverse set of deep neural networks (DNNs) optimized to perform different scene perception tasks. We found a structured mapping between DNN tasks and brain regions along the ventral and dorsal visual streams. Low-level visual tasks mapped onto early brain regions, 3-dimensional scene perception tasks mapped onto the dorsal stream, and semantic tasks mapped onto the ventral stream. This mapping was of high fidelity, with more than 60% of the explainable variance in nine key regions being explained. Together, our results provide a novel functional mapping of the human visual cortex and demonstrate the power of the computational approach. <<<
翻译
回到顶部